May 212014
 

Harel, Kravitz and Baker recently published an article in Frontiers in Human Neuroscience (2013), Beyond perceptual expertise: revisiting the neural substrates of expert object recognition, which was also a poster at VSS (May19th, 2014).

This is an opinion piece, and I believe that authors are entitled to their opinions, and that debate is generally good for progress in a field. However, under the pretense of proposing a novel framework (see Wong & Wong’s commentary on the article for an explanation that this is not in fact a novel framework), the authors have severely mis-represented the literature on expertise. Next year, at VSS, we will be celebrating the 30th meeting of the Perceptual Expertise Network: so I know this field, and I do not recognize it in the picture painted by Harel et al.

My point here is not to dispute the claims made in the paper. I find it more appropriate to do in empirical demonstrations, such as in this recent paper. Rather, I want to state that my views (and of course those of my co-authors) are misrepresented in this article. I could say ”misunderstood”, but after having discussed this with the first author, and going back to the literature record to make sure that I am not completely wrong about my own work, I think misrepresentation is a fair term.(this is probably too strong, Harel et al. swear they really thought these were appropriate representations of my/our ideas). And I do not believe that such distortion leads to clear debate that is generally good for a field. It is in nobody’s interest that people becoming interested in the study of expertise would be provided with such a distorted picture of the field. I embrace people disagreeing with my views, but only when they are really my views.

Let me focus on two main points:

1. “…attaining any form of visual expertise should be supported primarily by qualitative changes in processing within specific regions of visual cortex (Palmeri and Gauthier, 2004).”

Overall, in this paper (and others by the same authors) as well as in the poster, what we are being associated with is a simplistic theory, something like the main effect of “expertise” (any kind) is changes in one area (the FFA).

This is a strawman we would not want to be associated with. Let me emphasize this:

I would not argue that expertise with all categories are the same behaviorally, nor in the brain.

You will find evidence of this across all these articles:

• James, K. H., James, T. W., Jobard, G., Wong, C.-N., Gauthier, I. (2005). Letter processing in the visual system: different patterns for single letters and strings, Cognitive and Affective Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(4): 452-66. PDF
• James, K.H., Gauthier, I. (2006). Letter Processing automatically recruits a multimodal brain network, Neuropsychologia, 44(14):2937-49. PDF
• Wong, C.-N, Gauthier, I. (2007). An analysis of letter expertise in a levels-of-categorization framework, Visual Cognition, 15(7): 854-879. PDF
• Wong, A.C.-N., Jobard, G., James, K.H., James, T.W., Gauthier,I. (2009). Expertise with characters in alphabetic and non-alphabetic writing systems engage overlapping occipito-temporal areas, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(1), 111-127.PDF
• Wong, Y.K., Gauthier, I. (2010). A multimodal neural network recruited by expertise with musical notation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 695-713.PDF
• Wong, A. C.-N., Palmeri, T. J., Gauthier, I. (2009). Conditions for face-like expertise with objects: Becoming a Ziggerin expert – but which type? Psychological Science, 20(9): 1108-1117. PDF
• Wong, A.C.-N., Palmeri, T.J., Rogers, B.P., Gore, J.C., Gauthier, I. (2009). Beyond shape: How you learn about objects affects how they are represented in visual cortex, PLoS One, 2(12), e8405.PDF
• Wong, Y. K., Folstein, J. R., & Gauthier, I. (2012). The nature of experience determines object representations in the visual system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 141(4):682-698. PDF

Second, I do not believe that the primary result of expertise is necessarily selectivity in the FFA.

 I started my career asking why faces appeared to be special in specific ways. Why do they show holistic processing? Why do they engage the FFA? These are questions that some of our research specifically addresses. When trying to test the claim that faces are special because of a specific effect, it is more than appropriate to look at that effect/area. By showing that there is no clear dissociation (between faces and objects of expertise) in that effect/area, you address that specific claim. That’s it. It doesn’t make any prediction about expertise not having other behavioral or neural effects.

 Have we published studies focusing on the FFA’s role in expertise? For sure! But these particular papers address a very strong claim that is not ours: One that suggests face perception is “a cognitive function with its own private piece of real estate in the brain” (Kanwisher, 2000; 2010).  If there is a claim here that is “face-centric”, or “FFA-centric”, it is that original claim, which we are challenging.

Importantly, we have not only addressed this question. We have reported, starting in 1999 in our work on Greeble expertise, and in 2000 in our work on car and bird experts, other areas than the FFA that show expertise effects. We have continued to do so for different kinds of expertise since then (see papers cited above).

2.” …expert processing under this perceptual view is automatic and stimulus- driven, with little impact of attentional, task demands or other higher-level cognitive factors (Tarr and Gauthier, 2000; Palmeri et al., 2004).

The Wong & Wong commentary on the Harel et al. paper does a good job of outlining how those of us who have been studying perceptual expertise for years have been looking both at the automatization of processing with expertise, and have studied the interaction of perception with high-level factors.

For recent examples: our work on selectivity for musical note expertise (Wong et al., in press) specifically focuses on the influence of top-down influences; In Wong, Folstein & Gauthier (2012), where we compare perceptual expertise and perceptual learning, we discuss how experience interacts with attentional set to determine the pattern of training effects in the brain; and in McGugin et al., (2012), we discuss the possibility that semantic knowledge interacts with perceptual expertise to lead to a more bilateral representation.

Instead of calling those of us who have been studying expertise for more than 15 years “face-centric”, Harel et al. could have actually covered some of the work we (and others) have done that already goes well beyond faces and FFA. Instead they focused only on those papers that specifically address the theory that “FFA is a unique face module”, and ended up presenting a very skewed view of what this field has been all about.

I waited a while before “greebling” about this one – but Harel and colleagues seem intent on framing their own contributions against this face-centric view of the perceptual expertise literature. I am sending them the link to this clarification, and I hope that they stop misrepresenting my contributions as face-centric. If you have opinions about this, one way or another, I have posted a comment on the Wong & Wong opinion on the Frontiers web site, you can add yours.

References without links:

Kanwisher N (2000) Domain specificity in face perception. Nat Neurosci 3:759763

Kanwisher, N. (2010). Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind. PNAS107(25), 11163-11170.

 

 

May 212014
 

Gender effects for toy faces: quantitative differences in face processing strategies
Katlin Ryan and Isabel Gauthier
PDF

Does acquisition of holistic processing for novel objects depend on experience with diagnostic parts?
Kao-Wei Chua, Jenn Richler and Isabel Gauthier
PDF 

Measurement of semantic knowledge of object categories: creating the Semantic  Vanderbilt Expertise Test (SVET)
Ana E. Van Gulick and Isabel Gauthier
PDF

Holistic processing of faces in the composite task depends on size.
David A. Ross and Isabel Gauthier
PDF

Modeling the Moderation of Experience in Face and Object Recognition
Panqu Wang, Benjamin Cipollini, Akinyinka Omigbodun, Isabel Gauthier, Gary Cottrell
PDF 

“Greebling” on Harel, Kravitz & Baker (VSS2014)  

 

Apr 232014
 

Harrison, S., Gauthier, I., Hayward, W., Richler, J.J. Other-race effects manifest in overall performance, not qualitative processing style. Visual Cognition.

Apr 232014
 

…turns out that I have been told to consider a THIRD paper to be essentially accepted, even though I have not yet received the official letter. And because three papers in two days seems like a great feat, I am counting it even though I am being careful and not announcing the actual paper yet (Editors:  we have several papers under review now, feel free to compete for that third slot!).

Apr 202014
 

Sat May 17 12:45 pm. Individual differences brown bag.
Measurement of holistic processing for individual differences study
Isabel Gauthier, Jennifer J. Richler, David A. Ross

There is growing interest in the study of individual differences in face recognition, including one of its hallmarks, holistic processing, which can be defined as a failure of selective attention to parts, as measured in the composite task. We investigate the reliability of composite task measurements in eight datasets from five different samples of subjects (160 trials in each case; Ross, Richler and Gauthier, under review). Reliability was fairly low with substantial variability across experiments, with the highest reliability (.50-.67) using methods designed to increase reliability. Unlike DeGutis et al (2013), we do not find that regressing out the control condition (misaligned face halves) consistently produced more reliable results than subtracting it, although we agree subtraction is not ideal theoretically. Based on a recent meta-analysis (39 studies, N=1223), we find that the correlation between the congruency effects in the experimental (aligned) and control (misaligned) conditions is low, r = 24 (95% CI: .16, .32). We argue that for the purpose of individual differences analyses, when dealing with upright faces in samples of normal adult subjects, reliability can be maximized by collecting more data from aligned trials. Under such conditions, there is little need to regress out results in a control condition that shares very little variance with the aligned condition in the first place.

(#33.574)  Does acquisition of holistic processing for novel objects depend on experience with diagnostic parts?
C Kao-Wei, J Richler, I Gauthier
(Session: Face perception: Whole and parts – 8:30AM Sun May 18 – Poster Presentation)

– note that the abstract we submitted was based on analyses that included a small error which, when fixed, made a very important difference! Also, the first author is Kao-Wei Chua.

(#33.577)  Holistic processing of faces in the composite task depends on size
D Ross, I Gauthier
(Session: Face perception: Whole and parts – 8:30AM Sun May 18 – Poster Presentation)

(#43.511)  Measurement of semantic knowledge of object categories: Creating the Semantic Vanderbilt Expertise Test (SVET)
A Van Gulick, I Gauthier
(Session: Face perception: Experience, learning and expertise 1 – 8:30AM Mon May 19 – Poster Presentation)

(#43.513)  Gender effects for toy faces reveal qualitative differences in face processing strategies
K Ryan, I Gauthier
(Session: Face perception: Experience, learning and expertise 1 – 8:30AM Mon May 19 – Poster Presentation)

Apr 092014
 

In an attempt to boost her productivity (as if that was possible), Rankin let go of Brain Voyager just long enough to bring another lovely Rankin (this one, Nancy Rankin McGugin, yes people, pay attention) into this world. Huge hug and congratulations to Rankin and Bill!

Mar 272014
 

Van Gulick, A.E, Gauthier, I. (in press). The perceptual effects of learning object categories that predict perceptual goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition.

 

Mar 252014
 

Rezlescu, Barton, Pitcher & Duchaine  (RBPD) recently published an article in PNAS, presenting evidence that two patients with acquired prosopagnosia were able to become “Greeble experts”. RBPD argue that this refutes a fundamental prediction of the expertise hypothesis.

Although it is always interesting to hear about what specific patient can and cannot do, I would argue we cannot learn much that is compelling from this evidence. I was asked to comment on the article by Andreas von Bubnoff, a journalist for Nature News (see his coverage), but since my comments will likely be greatly abbreviated, I thought I would elaborate here.

Not an actual prediction.
RBPD argue that a “fundamental prediction” of the expertise hypothesis is that people with prosopagnosia should also be impaired at acquiring expertise for other objects. Although at first glance this may sound right, it is incorrect. The expertise account proposes that the reason we observe specialization for faces in behavior and in the brain is due to a specific kind of experience with faces. Or, to state it more specifically: if you have to individuate non-face objects that are visually similar and you use the same strategy most people use to learn faces, then the same kind of specialization may occur for non-face objects.

The qualifications are important, because expertise is not ONE thing. Expertise is just becoming really good in a given domain that was difficult to start with. My colleagues and I have published a considerable amount of work in which we show that how you become an expert constrains the kind of expert you become (e.g., McGugin et al., 2011; Wong et al., 2012; Wong et al., 2009a). Clearly one can become an expert in non-face domains, like cars, and recruit the face area (McGugin et al., 2012; in press; Xu, 2005) and in other domains, like print or musical notation, and engage other brain areas (e.g., Wong et al., 2009b; Wong et al., 2012). How you become an expert is likely to depend on a number of factors, including the information available in the stimuli, the task you are trying to perform, whatever biases you may bring to the situation, and what strategies are available to you.

This last point about strategies is really important to interpret RBPD’s new article. They used a procedure that was designed to elicit a specific strategy—holistic processing— in normal subjects, but their argument depends on the assumption that this is the only strategy one could use. We may be responsible for this misconception in not being sufficiently clear in our early work. However, our work with prosopagnosic patient LR (Bukach et al., 2012), who also became a Greeble expert like the patients in RBPD’s article, is very clear on this point. Because each Greeble has unique parts, there is no principled reason one has to use holistic processing in a Greeble training study. The fact that normal subjects seem to adopt a holistic processing strategy does not speak to what patients would do if integrating across parts is not easy for them. By testing controls and patient LR’s processing of Greebles that did not have unique parts after training, we were able to show that similar performance can come from different strategies. This was not verified by RBPD about their patients, so we cannot know whether their processing of Greebles is holistic and normal, just that their performance individuating Greebles is as good as controls.

Suggesting that Greebles and faces are somehow equated.
The critical evidence in RBPD’s article is that the patients can learn Greebles and not faces. Thus, whether the two tasks are equated is important. In their abstract, they called the face procedure “matched” to the Greeble task. But their description makes it clear that they selected faces to be highly similar, whereas they did not do the same for Greebles. They used 20 Greebles during training, 4 Greebles from each family (apparently 2 from each gender, defined by all parts pointing up or down), which means that each Greeble in the experiment only had a single similar foil (another Greeble with the same body shape and direction of parts). The original Greeble set used in the first Greeble training study (Gauthier & Tarr, 1997) and in the work with patient LR (Bukach et al., 2012) used a more difficult set with 30 Greebles (2 similar foils per object). The faces RBPD used are more homogenous than the Greebles on any metric we can think of (this is obvious from their figure).

Imagine that the deficit in prosopagnosia has to do with a difficulty integrating across parts of an object. Integrating across parts will be particularly useful when the local featural information is not very diagnostic. Nobody predicts that RBPD’s patients should have difficulty processing the overall body shape of these objects or the direction of their parts. Thus, the Greeble task requires RBPD’s patients to learn to distinguish each Greeble from a single foil, and this can be done by looking at a single part, say the top right appendage (because all Greebles have unique parts). In contrast, the Face task could be performed based on single parts but each face has 19 other similar foils). These two tasks are not matched in difficulty. This is apparent when you consider that the control subjects, who have had a lifetime of experience with faces and no experience with Greebles, did equally well at learning greebles as they did faces.

These properties should not be taken as limitations of the Greebles or the Greeble training procedure. These were designed on purpose to be easier than faces, as a much simplified reduced “world” in which a limited amount of training might be able to produce holistic processing and FFA activity. The training itself is a manipulation and was not designed as a test of any kind. Being able to learn the Greebles was never supposed to be a test of whether someone is using face-like processing (although how a patient may learn them might be telling, Bukach et al., 2012).

 

Bukach, C.M., Gauthier, I., Tarr, M.J., Kadlec, H., Barth, S., Ryan, E., Turpin, J.  & Bub, D. (2012). Does acquisition of expertise in prosopagnosia rule of out a domain-general deficit?, Neuropsychologia, 50(2): 289-304.

Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M.J. (1997). Becoming a “Greeble” expert: Exploring mechanisms for face recognition, Vision Research, 37(12), 1673-1682.

McGugin, R.W., Van Gulick, A.E., Tamber-Rosenau, B.J., Ross, D.A. & Gauthier, I. (in press). Expertise effects in face selective areas are robust to clutter and diverted attention but not to competition. Cerebral Cortex.

McGugin, R.W., Gatenby, Gore, J.C.,Gauthier, I. (2012). High-resolution imaging of expertise reveals reliable object selectivity in the FFA related to perceptual performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(42), 17063-17068.

McGugin, R.W., Tanaka, J.W., Lebrecht, S., Tarr, M.J., & Gauthier, I. (2011). Race-Specific perceptual discrimination improvement following short individuation training with faces. Cognitive Science, 35(2):330-47.

Wong, A. C.-N., Palmeri, T. J., Gauthier, I. (2009a). Conditions for face-like expertise with objects: Becoming a Ziggerin expert – but which type? Psychological Science. 20(9), 1108-17.

Wong, A.C.-N., Jobard, G., James, K.H., James, T.W., & Gauthier, I. (2009b). Expertise with characters in alphabetic and non-alphabetic writing systems engage overlapping occipito-temporal areas. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(1): 111-127.

Wong, Y.K., Folstein, J.R., & Gauthier, I. (2012). The nature of experience determines object representations in the visual system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4):662-98.

Wong, Y.K. & Gauthier, I. (2012). Music-reading expertise alters visual spatial resolution for musical notation, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(4):594-600.

Xu, Y. (2005). Revisiting the role of the fusiform face area in visual expertise.Cerebral Cortex15(8), 1234-1242.